Page Nav

HIDE

Grid

GRID_STYLE

intro

Breaking News

latest

  The underlying metacognitive component of self-regulation in some form or another is evident in other reviews (behavioral approaches appea...


 


The underlying metacognitive component of self-regulation in some form or another is evident in other reviews (behavioral approaches appear not to be represented in the more recent literature). McCombs (1984), in her review, focused on relationships between metacognitive components, perceptions of personal control that contributed to continuing motivation, and skills training interventions. She defined skills training in a very broad sense, however, as anything that may generate processes conducive to learning and that may become subject to intervention. Intervention steps include cognitive and metacognitive strategy training, and within the affective system she includes targets related to eliminating negative and creating positive self-views and making students aware of inappropriate self-cognitions. McCombs's "motivational skills training


" program is designed to focus on affective and process variables as outcomes rather than on performance per se; consequently, it is difficult to judge how effective the program is in enhancing learning in the conventional, institutional sense. Derry and Murphy (1986) explicitly take Bloom's (1984) "2 sigma" criterion; that is, to design whole-class interventions that would meet the level of achievement-two standard deviations above the norm-possible under the ideal instructional condition of one-to-one tutoring. They again use a strong top-down approach,


 developing a model of intervention from the theories of Gagne (1980), Steinberg (1983), and metacognitive theorists such as Flavell (1979) and A. L. Brown (1978) together with a review of intervention studies. They develop a taxonomy of intervention targets ranging from microcomponents or tactics, which are easily trainable, to executive components, which appear to develop only with much in situ practice in contexts and curricula that evoke and support them. The last position finds elaborated support from Perkins and Salomon (1989), who referred to the "low" and "high" roads to transfer-the former based precisely on specificity and long practice, and the latter on the deliberate and mindful abstraction of principles, and the search for analogies, that might link specific situations with each other. Haller, Child, and Walberg (1988) meta-analyzed 20 studies of metacognitive intervention in reading skills and found an average effect size of 0.71,


 which is impressive. Nevertheless, the success of such interventions is not universal, which prompted Garer (1990) to ask why people do not use learning strategies they have been taught to use. She concluded that training tends to remain situated; only exceptionally will students use strategies in contexts other than those in which they are taught. Pintrich and de Groot (1990) emphasized the motivational roots of transfer; students need the "will" as well as the "skill" in learning if they are to continue to use the strategies they have been taught. Thinking today has come a long way from the simple instruction in "study skills," and there is something of a theoretical consensus about the nature of interventions that might enhance learning. First, the target of intervention is not simply a tactic or microcomponent such as a particular study skill or set of study skills, as would have been the case 30 or 40 years ago, but rather a range of cognitive and metacognitive procedures. The catch is that the more general and more abstract these procedures, 


the harder it is to achieve measurable results of intervention (Derry & Murphy, 1986; Perkins & Salomon, 1989). Second, the matter is not only cognitive but also affective, involving motivation both as a precursor to effective strategy use (Biggs, 1987; Kirschenbaum & Perri, 1982; 102 This content downloaded on Sun, 3 Feb 2013 08:00:24 AM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions Effects of Study Skills Interventions Pintrich & de Groot, 1990) and as a continuing support to the complex of learningrelated beliefs and procedures (McCombs, 1984). Third, the teaching context should evoke, support, and maintain the components being targeted by intervention (Biggs, 1993; Derry & Murphy, 1986; Garner, 1990). Collectively, then, there are several suggested conditions for successful strategy training: (a) high and appropriate motivation, including self-efficacy and appropriate attributions (such as attributing failures to a lack of effort, and setting realistic and attainable goals); 


(b) the strategic and contextual knowledge for doing the task; and (c) a teaching-learning context that supports and reinforces the strategies being taught. At the present time, however, attempts at modeling intervention programs for enhanced learning lack broadly based supportive data. Not to put too fine a point on it, theory may have leapt ahead of the evidence. But even within the consensus referred to here, the relative effectiveness of a variety of programs and thrusts needs evaluating for both theoretical and practical reasons.

No comments

Ads